REM sleep

a kind of sleep that occurs at intervals during the night and is characterized by Rapid Eye Movements (REM), more dreaming and bodily movement, and faster pulse and breathing.

Scientists Communicate in Real Time With People Who Are Asleep

Name of Review Item: 
Real-time dialogue between experimenters and dreamers during REM sleep
Media Type: 
Paper
Author/Creator: 
Karen Konkoly, Kristoffer Appel, Emma Chabani, Anastasia Mangiaruga, Jarrod Gott, Remington Mallett, Bruce Caughran, Sarah Witkowski, Nathan Whitmore, Christopher Mazurek, Jonathan Berent, Frederik Weber, Başak Türker, Smaranda Leu-Semenescu
Name of Reviewer: 
Michael C. Willis
Date of Review: 
05/02/2022

Hello Subtle Realm readers,

This article could not be overlooked here at Subtle Realm. This is a big development in the area of dreams and sleep science. For years, scientists have been able to influence dreams using external means, but results have been spotty due to the inability of the subjects to recall their dreams (either accurately or at all) upon waking. And no experiments have involved the subjects communicating back to the experimenters while dreaming - in real time - until now.  

Here we have an article describing recent scientific experiments in which scientists have been able to successfully communicate in real time with people who are asleep. Here is what the experiment entailed in brief: 

A group of 36 people were selected. Participants were divided into three groups: (1) experienced lucid dreamers, (2) healthy people with no experience who were trained to lucid dream, and (3) a narcoleptic patient who reported frequent lucid dreaming. The scientists constructed a system of communication using taps, flashing lights, and/or sounds as external stimuli. The sleepers used a pre-arranged set of signals using unique eye movements and facial muscle contractions to report answers to the questions asked after the stimuli were administered. 

The experimenters waited until the sleepers were in REM sleep because lucid dreaming is strongly associated with the REM state. The sleepers were able to signal with eye movements or facial contractions when they were having a lucid dream, and that's when the experimenters began to interact with them. 

When the external cue was given (a flash of light for example), the sleeper was told in a soft voice to obtain complete lucidity in the moment and direct their attention to the voice, and then the voice immediately asked a question of the sleeper. The sleeping subject was to answer using the eye movements or muscle contractions which produce measurable electrical signals and were thus recorded by sensors attached to the subject. The questions asked were not given to the sleeper prior to going to sleep, and so neither the questions nor the answers became repetitive.

Here is a brief summary of the results. The dreamer reached REM sleep and conveyed he was dreaming lucidly using the pre-arranged signals. The experimenters verbally asked the question: "8 minus 6". Answer: "2". The question was repeated, the same answer given. In real time. Upon awakening (immediately after the question/answer exchange was recorded) the dreamer recounted his lucid dream fairly accurately to the experimenters. He recalled much of the verbal instructions verbatim, though there were some distortions in his post-dream interpretation. 

For information on what controls were used and other questions about how the experiment was conducted, please read the article in its entirety.

Is the passing of this small bit of information (the answer to the math problem: 'two') during an experiment between a sleeping subject and a wakeful scientist a big deal? While the amount of information may be small, it is monumental in the arena of sleep science, and in science in general! 

There has been a convention of thought around the condition of the human being during sleep - that we are closed off to anything but strong stimuli and are incapable of rational thought while our bodies remain inert for recharging. In light of how we are perceived when we wake up from sleep (grogginess), our difficulty remembering our dreams clearly, and our lack of knowledge regarding lucid dreaming, this is entirely expected. But in at least one mode of consciousness (lucidly dreaming REM sleep) this has been scientifically disproven as a result of these experiments. Humans have long known that consciousness remains active during sleep, but now that we have the results of this experiment, our understanding should be far more broad. Our attitudes toward sleep should change, especially since (on average) we spend about 37 percent of our lives asleep. 

The experiment showed that people still think and use their logical minds when their bodies are asleep. They were able to take in information not previously known to them, think about/process it, and give an answer to a problem that involved the information - all while asleep. This establishes that, at least while lucid dreaming, our consciousness is still active much like it is while awake. 

The fact that the subjects sometimes could not remember, or would remember a distorted version of the events that transpired during the experiment shows a limitation of consciousness that can be avoided if the communication with the sleeper is in real-time. This has long been a hindrance to studying sleep and sleep experiences scientifically. 

The implications of this research are vast! The creative nature of consciousness while dreaming is well known.

There have been many instances of artists who have admitted that a piece of art came to them in a dream. Inventors have come forward with similar stories. Could the next technological game-changer come from a lucid dreamer? Could artists be nudged into giving the world musical and visual masterpieces? Could we help lessen the pain caused by abuse? Could a scientist be gently guided to a discovery that would solve the energy crisis? Could we at last learn about the nature of consciousness itself?

Glossary Terms: 

You Snooze, You Might Not Lose

Michael C. Willis's picture

Most adults, during sleep, begin a dream period called REM sleep every 1.5 hours or so. During REM sleep, your Subtle World experiences (dreams) can get very intense. Breathing and heart rates go up and your physical brain is more active. According to William Buhlman in his book Adventures Beyond the Body, one of the best times to have an out-of-body experience (OBE) is early in the morning “after two or three REM periods (three to five hours) have passed”. He goes on to say, “The scientific correlation between REM periods and out-of-body experiences is yet to be confirmed; however, reports linking the two are commonplace”. 

My experience has been that I tend to remember dreams more often during the above mentioned times, and more generally, after waking up and going back to sleep. Sometimes I’ll wake up to roll over and get comfortable, sometimes it’s a bathroom visit, but typically I do find that Buhlman’s information is correct in that I remember my dreams more often at these times. If you are interested in a technique of his that utilizes these facts to induce out-of-body experiences, I highly suggest reading the book for details, but just know that it takes a fair amount of discipline to follow the instructions, but success in having an OBE is more likely. 

If you aren’t quite ready to dive into the deep end with that, here’s something to consider, as it doesn’t take much effort at all. I myself have had very vivid dreams after hitting the snooze button on the alarm clock. Yep - right after saying (sometimes out loud), “Ugh, just 10 more minutes!”. For most people this is a very mundane part of the day that rarely gets attention, but it could be the best part if you use your time wisely.

When you hit the snooze button on your alarm, phone, chicken, whatever the device, you will re-enter sleep with a consciousness that is just a little closer to the waking state by reason of you just having been awake moments ago. Your brain will be more active (and thus more aware) compared to the deep sleep you may experience just before a REM period. This is the perfect time to use your targeting technique to have an OBE. It may take some practice - be dedicated and your persistence will pay off.

Give it a shot. You have nothing to lose and everything to gain. If you meet with any success please post about it here and describe your experience to inspire and share with others!

Recalling and Forgetting Dreams: Theta and Alpha Oscillations during Sleep Predict Subsequent Dream Recall

Name of Review Item: 
Recalling and Forgetting Dreams: Theta and Alpha Oscillations during Sleep Predict Subsequent Dream Recall
Media Type: 
Paper
Author/Creator: 
Cristina Marzano, Michele Ferrara, Federica Mauro, Daniela Tempesta, Fabio Moroni, Maurizio Gorgoni, Carlo Cipolli, and Luigi De Gennaro
Name of Reviewer: 
Michael C. Willis
Date of Review: 
02-15-2019
Stars: 
4

This scientific study explores two hypotheses:

1. The physiological mechanisms we use to remember our dreams are the same mechanisms we use to remember things when we are awake.

2. The brain waves (sometimes called ’oscillations’) we emit during REM sleep are different than the oscillations we emit during non-REM sleep, and that these different oscillations can be predictive in dream recall when awoken from either REM or non-REM sleep.

This study did confirm and support hypothesis number 1. If theta waves (5 - 7.5 Hz) are present in higher amounts in the frontal cortex region of the brain when we are awoken from REM sleep, it is positively correlated to the ability to remember dreams. Similarly, theta waves present in higher power concentrations in the frontal regions of the brain when we commit something to memory (while we are awake) are predictive of whether we can remember it later. In the words of the scientists themselves: “our results suggest that the neuro-physiological mechanisms underlying the encoding and recall of episodic memories remain the same across different states of consciousness”, and that this “undoubtedly strengthens the notion of a continuity between waking and sleep mentation”. 

Hypothesis 2 was also confirmed and supported by the evidence gathered in the study. The subjects were awoken from both REM and non-REM sleep after the scientists determined that the test subjects’ state of consciousness had remained unchanged for 5 minutes prior to awakening. While higher frontal theta activity when awoken from REM sleep is predictive of dream recall, it is lower alpha wave activity in the right temporal region of the brain that is predictive of dream recall when awoken from non-REM sleep. Interestingly, other studies (cited in this paper) have shown that people can remember words and faces better when the encoding of those memories is accompanied by lower alpha activity in the right temporal region.

The results of the study provide clear scientific evidence that not only do our brains function the same (with regard to memory encoding and retrieval) during both wakefulness and sleep, but we can predict whether a person will remember something (dreamt or not) by the presence or absence of certain rhythms in the brain. 

I found this study to be of particular interest because the scientists are now exploring different states of consciousness during sleep. We know a great deal about REM sleep, since this seems to be the part of the sleep cycle when we are most active - our physical bodies seem to move without us being fully conscious of it and so it naturally captures our interest and attention. But to my knowledge, REM sleep and non-REM sleep have never before been scientifically compared and contrasted, especially with respect to brain waves which can tell us a lot about what is happening ‘under the hood’, so to speak. I applaud this endeavor and hope to see more of it in the future.

Glossary Terms: 

State- or trait-like individual differences in dream recall: preliminary findings from a within-subjects study of multiple nap REM sleep awakenings

Name of Review Item: 
State- or trait-like individual differences in dream recall: preliminary findings from a within-subjects study of multiple nap REM sleep awakenings
Media Type: 
Paper
Author/Creator: 
Serena Scarpelli, Cristina Marzano, Aurora D’Atri, Maurizio Gorgoni, Michele Ferrara, and Luigi De Gennaro
Name of Reviewer: 
Michael C. Willis
Date of Review: 
01/28/2019
Stars: 
5

This study delves into the ability of people to recall their dreams with the aim of determining whether dream recall is based on “state- versus trait-like factors”. In other words, is the ability to recall dreams more dependent upon what state the brain is in just before awakening (state), or is it more based upon the brainwave patterns emitted by an individual on an ongoing basis (trait)? What are the factors that lead up to being able to remember one’s dreams? 

The first point made is that an increase in the same frontal brainwave patterns present (theta waves) prior to being able to recall previously learned information during wakefulness are also increased just prior to being able to recall dreams when awoken during the study. This indicates that the state of mind before the beginning of a task is very important to memory performance relative to that task. As a result of these observations, this team of scientists were able to predict with a high degree of accuracy whether an individual will recall their dreams upon awakening, and how often this will occur. 

These findings, although significant, did not answer the state- versus trait-like question. 

The scientists were able to discover and isolate a kind of “background” signal from individuals’ brainwave patterns, and so were then able to study the other signals (electroencephalogram or EEG oscillations), apart from this during rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep. One can think of this “background” signal as the steady “trait” of an individual, and the EEG oscillations as the passing “state” of the individual’s brainwave patterns at a given time. Thus, once the scientists were able to separate these two signals, they were able to set up an experiment to determine which one had more influence on memory recall during wakefulness and sleep. 

The conclusion of the study indicates that the experiment not only identifies the presence of heightened right-frontal theta activity, a state-like phenomenon, to be predictive of dream recall. It also further supports the hypothesis of the continuity of neurophysiological processes across wakefulness and sleep with regard to the “encoding and retrieval of episodic memory”. Or, simply put, the same brainwaves predict memory recall for both wakefulness and sleep, suggesting that our consciousnesses do not cease while our physical bodies are asleep.